Efficacy of an Autobiographical Memory Specificity Training for the Reduction of Depressive Symptomatology
- Conditions
- Training GroupControl Group
- Interventions
- Behavioral: Autobiographical Memory Specificity Training PESCAR
- Registration Number
- NCT05220618
- Lead Sponsor
- University of Valencia
- Brief Summary
The effect of Autobiographical Memory Specificity Training on memory specificity, positive affect and depression symptomatology
- Detailed Description
The general objective of this study is to analyze the efficacy of an online-delivered Autobiographical Memory Specificity Training to increase memory specificity, positive affect and to reduce depression symptomatology. It will be tested in two conditions (training group vs. control group) with low-to-moderate depressive symptomatology. It is hypothesized that the training group (over control group) will increase the specificity and positive affect of autobiographical memories, and this will lead to a reduction of depressive symptomatology.
Recruitment & Eligibility
- Status
- UNKNOWN
- Sex
- All
- Target Recruitment
- 42
- Age between 18 and 55 (following Serrano et al., 2007 recommendations)
- Deficit in memory specificity: scores on Autobiographical Memory Test (AMT) < (or equal) 70%
- Diagnosis of mild to moderate depression: scores on Patient Health Questionnaire (PHQ9) between 5 to 14
- being currently under psychological treatment
- present any medical illness or physical, psychological and/or cognitive incapacity that could impede the participation
- Not having the necessary technological means, i.e. access to a mobile phone with internet connection.
Study & Design
- Study Type
- INTERVENTIONAL
- Study Design
- PARALLEL
- Arm && Interventions
Group Intervention Description Memory Training Group Autobiographical Memory Specificity Training PESCAR This group will receive an online two-week intervention to train the specificity of autobiographical memories. During the two-week intervention and the following two weeks, participants will complete ecological-momentary assessments (EMA) to monitor the amount of positive and specific memories recalled during the day. EMA is an Experience Sampling Method assessment system that allows data to be collected from participants in their natural environment at various points in time (McDevitt-Murphy et al., 2018). For this study, we designed an EMA to monitor essential project variables throughout the intervention (2 weeks; from Day 0 to Day 15) and then as a follow-up (2 weeks after; from Day 16 to Day 30).
- Primary Outcome Measures
Name Time Method Autobiographical memory specificity. The Autobiographical Memory Test (AMT, Williams & Broadbent, 1986) Screening for eligibility criteria; Change in AMT from pre- intervention (Day 0) to post-intervention (Day 15) AMT is the preferred measure to evaluate the degree of specificity of autobiographical memory. During the AMT application, nine verbal cue words of different valence (three positive, three negative and three neutral) will be presented to the participants to perform a recall task of specific personal memories (Dritschel et al., 2014). The specificity of the memories is scored by evaluators following specified criteria (Williams et al., 2007), and then, an overall estimate of the level of specificity of autobiographical memories can be calculated. The assessment will be conducted online, following previous works with the written version of the AMT (Takano et al., 2017). The spanish adaptation (Ros et al., 2018) has shown adequate psychometric properties in young and older populations.
- Secondary Outcome Measures
Name Time Method Depressive symptomatology. The Patient Health Questionnaire 9 (PHQ-9; Kroenke, Spitzer & Williams, 2001, Spanish adaptation Diez-Quevedo et al., 2001) Change in PHQ-9 from pre- intervention (Day 0) to post-intervention (Day 15); Change in PHQ-9 from post- intervention (Day 15) to follow-up (Day 30) PHQ-9 is a nine-item self-report measure of depressive symptoms. Participants answer the statements using a four-point Likert scale (0= Not at all to 3=Nearly every day) (Kroenke, Spitzer \& Williams, 2001). The Spanish version has shown comparable diagnostic validity to the original English version, presenting levels of 88% of sensitivity and 88% of specificity (Diez-Quevedo et al., 2001).
Savoring. Ways of Savoring Checklist (WOSC, Bryant & Veroff, 2007; Jose, Lim, & Bryant, 2012) Change in WOSC from pre- intervention (Day 0) to post-intervention (Day 15); Change in WOSC from post- intervention (Day 15) to follow-up (Day 30) An abbreviated 19-item os WOSC assess the use of thoughts and behaviours that facilitate savoring by amplifying positive feelings (11 items; for example, "I thought about sharing the memory of this later with other people") and the use of thoughts and behaviours that inhibit savoring by dampening positive emotions (eight items; for example, "I told myself why I didn't deserve this good thing"). Participants indicated to what extent statements described their thoughts and behaviours during positive experiences over the past week (1 = definitely doesn't apply, 7 = definitely applies). Previous studies have reported good psychometric properties with αs = .85-.86 (Smith and Hanni, 2019).
Characteristics of positive memories recalled. (Continuous assessments) Daily mood and characteristics of positive memories during intervention (From Day 1 to day 15). Continuous assessment involves repeated sampling of subjects' behaviours and experiences in real time and in their natural environments, minimizing recall bias, maximizing ecological validity and allowing the study of micro-processes that influence behaviour. Previous studies have demonstrated the utility and validity of this assessment strategy in a variety of clinical and subclinical contexts (Colombo et al., 2020a, b).For this study, participants will record on their smartphones pre- and post- practice session measures. Pre-practice, participants will rate their mood (i.e. level of positive and negative affect). Post-practice, participants will rate their mood, and characteristics of the memory (i.e. level of specificity and positivity) and the recalling process (i.e. level of savoring and quality of the memory recall). This measure will be taken once a day, everyday during intervention (14 days: From Day 1 to day 15) and at follow-up (14 days; from Day 16 to Day 30)
Positive and negative affect. The Positive and Negative Affect Scale (PANAS; Watson et al., 1988; Sandín et al., 1999) Screening for eligibility criteria; Change in PANAS from pre- intervention (Day 0) to post-intervention (Day 15); Change in PANAS from post- intervention (Day 15) to follow-up (Day 30) A self-report measure consisting of two sub-scales that assess the person's positive and negative affect. Each scale is composed of 10 items, giving a total of 20 items on a 5-point Likert scale (1=not at all/very mild; 5=extreme). The PANAS is established as a scale to measure changes in mood with excellent psychometric properties in the general population in its Spanish version (Sandín et al., 1999). In addition, its reliability and validity has recently been evaluated in the assessment of people with emotional disorders. It has shown good to excellent internal consistency in both subscales (Cronbach's alpha for positive affect = 0.91; Cronbach's alpha for negative affect = 0.87).
Well-being. Mental Health Continuum Scale (MHC: Keyes, 2004; Echevarría et al., 2010) Screening for eligibility criteria; Change in MHC from pre- intervention (Day 0) to post-intervention (Day 15); Change in MHC from post- intervention (Day 15) to follow-up (Day 30) MHC-LF is a multidimensional measure, composed of 14 items measuring emotional (3 items), social (5 items) and psychological (6 items) well-being. It assesses the frequency with which they felt a certain way during the last month. Psychometric analyses of the Spanish validation (Echevarría et al., 2010) showed that it was a scale with good psychometric properties.
Trial Locations
- Locations (1)
University of Valencia
🇪🇸Valencia, Conunidad Valencia, Spain