Language Abilities of Children With ADHD
- Conditions
- ADHD
- Interventions
- Other: Language and neuropsychological tests
- Registration Number
- NCT03917316
- Lead Sponsor
- University of Aarhus
- Brief Summary
BACKGROUND: Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity-Disorder (ADHD) is associated with language difficulties within expressive, receptive and pragmatic areas of language.Telling stories is a particularly interesting language task as is provides the possibility of investigating how children use their language in everyday interactions. Thus, investigating how children with ADHD tell stories provides a more naturalistic impression of their language abilities. However, the extent, causes, and clinical relevance of language and narrative difficulties in ADHD remain largely unknown. Language may be the key to understanding and improving the functional impairments associated with ADHD such as social and academic function.
AIM: The overall aim of the current study was to investigate the association between language, narrative ability and ADHD, what contributes to this association, as well as the clinical relevance of language difficulties in ADHD in relation to academic and social function.
METHOD: Children between 7-11 years old with ADHD will be recruited from Aarhus University Hospital in Skejby and compared to children without ADHD recruited from schools in the community. All children will be assessed at two separate meetings at the Department of Psychology, Aarhus University. The total number of participants in each group will depend on the intake of patients at Aarhus University Hospital, but a maximum of 100 children will be sampled in each group.
- Detailed Description
BACKGROUND:
Up to 40% of children with the ADHD are suggested to have language difficulties. Other than language difficulties being debilitating in their own right, language difficulties in childhood ADHD are problematic because they likely enhance the social and academic difficulties often present in children with this disorder. Children with ADHD also appear to have narrative difficulties; i.e. they have problems telling a coherent, understandable story. Telling stories is a particularly interesting language task as the investigation of how children use their language in an unstructured task is similar to how they use their language in everyday interactions.
However, the extent, causes, and clinical relevance of language and narrative difficulties in ADHD remain largely unknown. Studies are scarce and fraught with problems warranting investigations of language abilities in children with ADHD that measure language as well as executive function (EF) and that investigate the clinical relevance of these language difficulties. This is necessary in order to tease apart and gain knowledge of the association between language, EF and ADHD. Ultimately, such an investigation can inspire future assessment and treatment of language difficulties in children with ADHD.
AIMS AND HYPOTHESES
1. The first aim is to examine narrative abilities in children with ADHD. This is done by investigating whether children with and without ADHD differ with respect to their narratives.
2. The second aim is to examine whether language, EF and/or ADHD symptoms can explain narrative function in children with and without ADHD.
3. The third aim is to investigate which specific domains of language children with ADHD have the most difficulty within.
4. The fourth aim is to investigate whether the language difficulties as well as the narrative difficulties identified are associated with academic and social function in ADHD.
PARTICIPANTS AND PROCEDURES Up to 100 children with ADHD, age 7-11, consecutively referred from Aarhus University Hospital, Denmark, will be compared to up to 100 typically developing children, age 7-11, recruited from schools in the same community as the hospital.
After initial screening by telephone, informed consent has been obtained from both custodians and verbal assent has been given by the child, all children (ADHD and control) will be assessed at two separate meetings at the University of Aarhus by a member of the project group. The parents will receive questionnaires about their child and if the families give their consent, questionnaires will also be sent to the children's primary teacher. The parents and teachers receive questionnaires about the child's behavior, language and social and academic abilities.
All hypotheses will be investigated with appropriate statistical analyses. The role of IQ, parental educational level etc. is also examined in the analyses. If an adequate sample size is obtained, confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) will be applied to handle data, and if a factor solution is supported, factor scores will be included in all analyses, thereby reducing the number of variables in the analyses.
REGISTRATION DETAILS The study record reported at clinical trials.gov is completely consistent with the protocol approved by the Central Region Committee on Health Research Ethics in Denmark. The study was registered at clinical trials.gov after enrollment had begun, but no changes have been made in any procedures, recruitment etc., and as such it was assessed to be feasible to include the few participants that had been enrolled before registration in clinical trials.
Recruitment & Eligibility
- Status
- COMPLETED
- Sex
- All
- Target Recruitment
- 97
Not provided
Not provided
Study & Design
- Study Type
- OBSERVATIONAL
- Study Design
- Not specified
- Arm && Interventions
Group Intervention Description ADHD Language and neuropsychological tests - Control Language and neuropsychological tests -
- Primary Outcome Measures
Name Time Method Working memory 2back task One day A self-designed 2back task modelled from Friedman et al., 2008). Measures the child's working memory. Outcome is mean accuracy, range 0-1, with higher scores indicating better working memory.
Inhibition Go/No Go task One day Go/No go (Tsujimoto, 2006), outcome percent false alarms, higher percents indicating problems within inhibition.
Working memory Tic Tac Toe task One day Tic Tac Toe (Huizinga et al., 2006). Measures the child's working memory. Outcome is mean accuracy, range 0-1, with higher scores indicating better working memory.
Inhibition Stop-signal task One day Stop-Signal task (Logan 1994; Logan, Schachar \& Tannock, 1997; Williams, Pronesse, Schachar; Logan \& Tannock, 1999); outcome Stop-signal Reaction time (SSRT), with slower reaction times indicating problems with inhibtion.
Academic abilities One day 5-15 questionnaire (Kadesjö, Janols, Korkman, Mickelsson, Strand, Trillingsgaard \& Gillberg, 2005). Questions from the learning domain. The parent rates the child on a scale of 1-3 on 29 questions about academic ability, with higher scores indicating academic difficulties.
Executive functions One day Childhood Executive Functioning inventory (CHEXI; Nyberg \& Thorell, 2008). Questionnaire. The parent rates the child on a scale of 1-5 on 26 questions concerning the childs executive functioning. Higher scores indicate problems within executive functioning.
General language ability One day Clinical Evaluation of Language Fundamentals-4 (Semel, Wiig \& Secord, 2003), measures general language ability. Rawscores on the task are converted to scale scores with a range of 1-14. A scale score of 7-10 is average, above indicates better language proficiency.
Pragmatic language ability One day Children's Communication Checklist-2 (CCC-2; Bishop, 2002), index for pragmatic ability, scaled scores (0-14), where 7-10 is average and above indicates better pragmatic language ability.
Narrative abilities One day Online story-telling of the wordless picture book "Frog, where are you"
Social abilities One day Social Competence Inventory-2 (Rydell Hagekull \& Bohlin, 1997) a questionnaire about social competencies. The parents rate their child on a scale of 1-5 on 25 questions, higher scores indicate better social ability.
Working memory Mental Counters task One day Mental Counters (Huizinga et al., 2006). Measures the child's working memory. Outcome is mean accuracy, range 0-1, with higher scores indicating better working memory.
Academic abilities teacher One day Teacher Telephone Interview (TTI, Tannock, Manassis \& Fung, 2003, revised and translated with permission from R. Tannock). The teacher rates the child on a scale of 1-5 on 9 school subjects. Higher scores indicate better academic ability.
Inhibition Flanker task One day Flanker task (Huyser et al., 2011), outcome median reaction time on conflict trials, with higher median reaction times indicating problems within inhibition.
- Secondary Outcome Measures
Name Time Method General cognitive abilities One day Ravens Coloured Matrices (Raven, 1998). A task that measures the child's general cognitive ability on a scale of 0-36, where higher scores indicate better cognitive abilities.
Reaction time One day Reaction time task (Tsujimoto, 2006) measures the child's average reaction time, with higher scores indicating faster reaction time.
Trial Locations
- Locations (1)
Department of Psychology, Aarhus University
🇩🇰Aarhus, (non-US), Denmark